Pragmatics: Crash Course Linguistics #6 - Free Educational videos for Students in K-12 | Lumos Learning

Pragmatics: Crash Course Linguistics #6 - Free Educational videos for Students in k-12


Pragmatics: Crash Course Linguistics #6 - By CrashCourse



Transcript
00:0-1 Hello . I'm taylor and welcome to crash course linguistics
00:02 . Sometimes we don't say exactly what we mean and
00:04 yet we still managed to understand each other if you
00:07 ask , is it raining when I come inside soaking
00:09 wet and I say great job Sherlock . You'll probably
00:12 assume that I'm being sarcastic rather than giving you a
00:14 compliment . Or if you ask me , can you
00:16 close the window ? I'll probably interpret your question as
00:19 a polite request rather than a question about my physical
00:22 ability . The reason we can figure out what's going
00:24 on is because we don't just look at words and
00:26 sentences for meaning . We also look at context .
00:29 The area of linguistics that puts meaning into context is
00:32 called pragmatics . Mm . Mhm . We don't have
00:46 100% complete information about everything that's going on when we're
00:49 talking to people . So we often need to make
00:51 some assumptions about the context in order to understand each
00:55 other . There are four main assumptions that pragmatic cysts
00:57 talk about when it comes to communication . Let's start
01:00 with great job Sherlock in some context . That could
01:03 be a statement of admiration at your friend's deductive powers
01:06 . But in other context , like if your friend
01:08 has done something especially unwise calling them , Sherlock actually
01:13 illustrates how much they're not like Sherlock Holmes . That's
01:16 because most of the time we assume that people are
01:18 trying to communicate high quality information , we know that
01:22 people can lie , but we usually assume that they're
01:25 telling the truth . So when the context and the
01:27 words clearly don't match , we can deduce a more
01:30 subtle truth like sarcasm . Let's move on to a
01:32 second assumption . Here is a gift that floating around
01:35 the Internet a while back with the caption . Look
01:37 at all these ducks , there are at least 10
01:40 . This caption is technically true . There are at
01:43 least 10 ducks . In fact , there's a whole
01:45 swarm of ducks , Probably hundreds And hundreds is definitely
01:49 consistent with at least 10 . But anyone who can
01:51 see that there are at least 10 ducks in this
01:54 shift can also see there are way more than 10
01:57 ducks and there's something so funny about the way that
02:00 the caption goes against our assumptions about communication . That
02:03 assumption is that people are giving us a sufficient quantity
02:06 of information , enough detail , but not too much
02:09 . The boring consistent with our assumption version of this
02:11 caption would have been , look at all these ducks
02:14 , There are hundreds , but the ordinary version wouldn't
02:17 have been as funny and probably wouldn't have gone as
02:20 viral . Food labels also generally align with our third
02:22 assumption . For example , if a pack of gum
02:24 says it's sugar free , it's because gums sometimes does
02:28 contain sugar , we generally assume that people will tell
02:30 us information that is of relevance . So the boring
02:33 gum packaging checks out . But our assumption about relevance
02:35 can also be used for humor or to mislead to
02:38 imply that something is relevant when it's actually not .
02:40 Like if an olive oil brand starts labelling , it's
02:43 bottles sugar free olive oil , you might think ,
02:46 wait a sec . I didn't know olive oil ever
02:48 contains sugar that might convince you to avoid other brands
02:51 of olive oil . That don't say they're sugar free
02:53 , even though none of them ever contain sugar ,
02:56 That information actually isn't relevant . Finally , let's say
02:59 you're trying to figure out whether you want to take
03:00 a particular class with a particular professor next year ,
03:03 you ask one person for advice . Well , it
03:06 certainly is a class . They say , you ask
03:09 someone else who says , Oh yeah , the professor
03:11 shows up every week and wears clothes and stands in
03:13 front of the room and talks to us and gives
03:16 us assignments . Both of these statements theoretically seem like
03:19 they should be completely unremarkable . Of course you'd expect
03:22 a class to be a class or a professor to
03:25 show up and wear clothing and give assignments . And
03:28 yet somehow , when your friends give you way less
03:30 detail than expected or lots of detail about obvious things
03:34 , it raises suspicions . What on earth is going
03:36 on with this class that they can't just tell you
03:38 if it's good or fourth assumption is that people will
03:40 say things in a manner which is as straightforward as
03:42 possible for the context . If something is good ,
03:45 we can probably just say that it's good . If
03:47 something is not so great though , we might be
03:49 reluctant to criticize it overtly . So we sometimes say
03:52 things in a less straightforward manner in order to be
03:55 more diplomatic . So when our friends say something that
03:57 miss aligns with our assumptions , that might tell us
03:59 that something's up with that Professor , these four assumptions
04:02 that what someone says will be of sufficient quality ,
04:05 quantity , relevance and manner can be summed up with
04:08 one bigger idea that we assume people are generally trying
04:11 to be cooperative with us . So these assumptions are
04:13 called the cooperative principle . They were first described by
04:16 the philosopher paul Grace . So they're also sometimes known
04:19 as Grace's maxims , but it's okay we can use
04:21 them to according to the co operative principle . Whenever
04:24 someone says something that doesn't make sense at a literal
04:27 level , we can figure out or infer what else
04:29 they could have meant assuming they're still trying to contribute
04:32 in a cooperative way to the conversation . Sometimes we
04:34 assume cooperation so quickly that we don't even really notice
04:37 it . For instance , if I say , hey
04:39 God , do you want to have a picnic and
04:40 gave says it's raining ? We can probably infer that
04:43 God was declining my picnic suggestion . But technically speaking
04:47 , God didn't actually say yes or no if we
04:50 were a computer program or a lawyer , someone else
04:52 who cares about very strict literal interpretations , we have
04:56 to point out that the picnic sentence and the reigning
04:58 sentence don't have to be related . It's only because
05:01 we have an understanding of context and cooperation that we
05:04 interpret them as related . We know that picnics involve
05:07 eating food outdoors and that it's hard to eat outdoors
05:09 in the rain . This additional meaning , layered on
05:12 top of the words were saying is known as an
05:14 implicate chur . Understanding how implicates her works can help
05:17 us make sense of the moments when someone says one
05:19 thing and means another . If God asks , can
05:21 I have a cookie and I reply , I don't
05:23 know , can you gavel quite justifiably be annoyed at
05:27 me because I'm deliberately ignoring the implicate chur that this
05:30 is a request and if you watch enough youtube videos
05:32 , you know what the subscribe button looks like and
05:34 that it sits below this video . So if I
05:36 turn to the camera and say the subscribe button is
05:39 below this video , I'm not actually telling you new
05:41 information , I'm not even asking you to subscribe ,
05:45 but you might have thought about it in this context
05:47 , pointing out the button is really saying please subscribe
05:50 without overtly saying please subscribe . I'm using implicate chur
05:54 to ask without asking . It's a way of being
05:56 polite by being in direct and languages have lots of
05:59 other strategies for being polite . Some language is at
06:02 a short word or particle to make something more polite
06:04 , like please are sorry in malay , you can
06:07 add law to a command , something like Henry ,
06:09 that law that turns it from a demand into something
06:12 more like would you please do that in mandarin ,
06:14 you tell a person to have a seat by just
06:16 saying sit sure , that probably sounds way too strong
06:19 like something you command your pet and it found strong
06:22 to mandarin speakers too . But instead of adding please
06:25 , they repeat the word tool , tool or sit
06:28 , sit , which means something like here have a
06:30 seat . Some languages have different forms of verbs or
06:32 other words depending on the social status of the person
06:35 that you're talking to in french the pronoun to is
06:39 informal and singular and vu is formal and plural .
06:42 English . Actually also used to make this distinction with
06:45 vow for the informal singular and you for the formal
06:48 or plural version of the word , making something seem
06:50 more questions like or tentative can also make it more
06:53 polite . In BSL raised eyebrows are used both to
06:56 indicate questions and also as a way of making a
06:59 request or apology more polite . While there's a wide
07:02 variety of grammatical ways to show politeness across languages ,
07:05 we also see a general tendency that adding qualifiers and
07:09 caveats known as hedges to our replies tend to be
07:12 seen as more polite . So does being indirect ,
07:14 such as asking or even just hinting rather than ordering
07:17 . So if I want you to close the window
07:19 because I'm freezing , it would be more polite to
07:21 say , would you mind closing the window or birds
07:25 chilly in here ? Then it would be just straight
07:27 up tell you close the window , we follow our
07:29 culture and our language is norms of politeness because it's
07:32 part of the whole process by which we create meaning
07:34 between us and the people were talking to both politeness
07:37 and the co operative principle are part of pragmatics .
07:40 They're part of our agreement about how we're going to
07:42 talk to each other . Pragmatics affects everything from our
07:45 words to even the very way we have those conversations
07:49 . Let's step into the thought bubble for a chat
07:51 . The flow of words between people is known as
07:53 turn taking . I say something you reply . I
07:57 reply to that back and forth . There's a lot
07:59 of variation across individuals and even across cultures as to
08:02 who does more or less of the speaking , how
08:05 long they talk for and how much overlap or silence
08:08 . There is between people talking when it comes to
08:10 overlap in conversation , we can think broadly about two
08:13 different ends of a spectrum . On one end ,
08:15 we have a conversation style where people do a lot
08:17 of overlapping talk at the same time and don't leave
08:20 much room for any silence after the other person is
08:23 finished speaking . This is known as high involvement interaction
08:26 style . On the other end we have a conversational
08:29 style where people do not overlap and leave space after
08:32 someone else's finished before beginning their turn . This is
08:35 known as high considerate Nous interaction style . You might
08:38 tend to be more high involvement and have a friend
08:40 who is high considerate nous and it makes you feel
08:42 like you're doing all the conversational , heavy lifting .
08:45 If you tend towards high considerate nous , you might
08:47 feel like your friend doesn't let you get a word
08:49 in edgewise , there's some general trends in different areas
08:52 to speakers of celta and Japanese tend to have more
08:55 overlap than speakers of lao and danish . Even within
08:58 american english . New Yorkers tend to be more high
09:00 involvement and Californians tend to be more high considerate nous
09:03 . These differences are measured in just milliseconds which shows
09:06 how sensitive humans can be to turn taking differences and
09:09 all the other little pragmatic nuances that make up politeness
09:12 . Thanks thought bubble , I appreciate it . So
09:14 when we look at how people use language and conversation
09:17 , we see that it's less like one person baking
09:20 in all the meaning they want to convey in their
09:21 words and handing it over to another person and more
09:24 like we're using context to bake a cake as a
09:26 team and just like everyone has their own way of
09:29 making a carrot cake . The individual conversation styles and
09:33 cultural norms mean each conversation or interaction can turn out
09:36 a little bit differently . Next time on crash course
09:39 linguistics will look specifically at how your social reality affects
09:42 your linguistic choices . Thanks for watching this episode of
09:45 crash course linguistics . If you want to help keep
09:47 all crash course for you for everybody forever you can
09:51 join our community on Patreon
Summarizer

DESCRIPTION:

OVERVIEW:

Pragmatics: Crash Course Linguistics #6 is a free educational video by CrashCourse.

This page not only allows students and teachers view Pragmatics: Crash Course Linguistics #6 videos but also find engaging Sample Questions, Apps, Pins, Worksheets, Books related to the following topics.


GRADES:


STANDARDS:

Are you the Publisher?

EdSearch WebSearch